Read Just Babies Online

Authors: Paul Bloom

Just Babies (25 page)

  
21
  
a baby’s “naive physics”:
See, for example, R. Baillargeon, “Object Permanence in 3½ and 4½ Month Old Infants,”
Developmental Psychology
23 (1987): 655–64; E. Spelke, “Principles of Object Perception,”
Cognitive Science
14 (1990): 29–56. For a review, see E. S. Spelke and K. D. Kinzler, “Core Knowledge,”
Developmental Science
10 (2007): 89–96.
  
22
  
babies can also do rudimentary math:
K. Wynn, “Addition and
Subtraction by Human Infants,”
Nature
358 (1992): 749–50. For a review of extensions and replications, see K. van Marle and K. Wynn, “Quantitative Reasoning,” in
Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science
, ed. Lynn Nadel (London: Nature Publishing Group, Macmillan, 2002). For a study of babies’ understanding of ratios, see K. McCrink and K. Wynn, “Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month-Old Infants,”
Psychological Science
18 (2007): 740–46.
  
23
  
They like the sound of human voices … they like the look of human faces:
For review, see Paul Bloom,
Descartes’ Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human
(New York: Basic Books, 2004).
  
24
  
how to freak out a baby:
E. Tronick, H. Als, L. Adamson, S. Wise, and T. B. Brazelton, “The Infant’s Response to Entrapment Between Contradictory Messages in Face-to-Face Interaction,”
Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry
17 (1978): 1–13.
  
25
  
In one study:
T. Field, N. Vega-Lahar, F. Scafidi, and S. Goldstein, “Effects of Maternal Unavailability on Mother-Infant Interactions,”
Infant Behavior and Development
9 (1986): 473–78; Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, and Brazelton, “Infant’s Response to Entrapment.”
  
26
  
babies know that individuals have goals:
A. Woodward, “Infants Selectively Encode the Goal of an Actor’s Reach,”
Cognition
69 (1998): 1–34.
  
27
  
fifteen-month-olds:
K. H. Onishi and R. Baillargeon, “Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs?,”
Science
308 (2005): 255–58.
  
28
  
previous work by the psychologists David Premack and Ann Premack:
D. Premack and A. J. Premack, “Infants Attribute Value +/- to the Goal-Directed Actions of Self-Propelled Objects,”
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
9 (1997): 848–56.
  
29
  
we created animations in which geometrical figures helped or hindered one another:
V. Kuhlmeier, K. Wynn, and P. Bloom, “Attribution of Dispositional States by 9-Month-Olds: The Role of Faces,” under review; V. Kuhlmeier, K. Wynn, and P. Bloom, “Attribution of Dispositional States by 12-Month-Old Infants,”
Psychological Science
14 (2003): 402–8; J. K. Hamlin, K. Wynn, and P. Bloom, “Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants,”
Nature
450 (2007): 557–59. To see examples of what the babies are shown, go
to “Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants,” 2007,
www.yale.edu/infantlab/socialevaluation/Helper-Hinderer.html
.
  
30
  
Our first set of studies used … puppets instead of animations:
Hamlin, Wynn, and Bloom, “Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants.”
  
31
  
We then followed this up with a pair of studies looking at three-month-olds:
J. K. Hamlin, K. Wynn, and P. Bloom, “3-Month-Olds Show a Negativity Bias in Social Evaluation,”
Developmental Science
13 (2010): 923–39.
  
32
  
a “negativity bias”:
A. Vaish, T. Grossmann, and A. Woodward, “Not All Emotions Are Created Equal: The Negativity Bias in Social-Emotional Development,”
Psychological Bulletin
134 (2008): 383–403; P. Rozin and E. Royzman, “Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion,”
Personality and Social Psychology Review
5 (2001): 296–320.
  
33
  
a project led by Mariko Yamaguchi:
M. Yamaguchi, V. Kuhlmeier, K. Wynn, and K. van Marle, “Continuity in Social Cognition from Infancy to Childhood,”
Developmental Science
12 (2009): 746–52.
  
34
  
Kiley and Karen created different sets of morality plays:
J. K. Hamlin and K. Wynn, “Five- and 9-Month-Old Infants Prefer Prosocial to Antisocial Others,”
Cognitive Development
26 (2011): 30–39.
  
35
  
identifying the helper as nice and the hinderer as mean:
J. K. Hamlin, K. Wynn, and P. Bloom, “Social Evaluation by Preverbal Infants,” poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, 2007.
  
36
  
Adam Smith … describes the moral sense:
Smith,
Theory of Moral Sentiments
, 222.

2. EMPATHY AND COMPASSION

    
1
  
some unhappy combination of genes, parenting, and idiosyncratic personal experience:
E. Viding, R. J. R. Blair, T. E. Moffitt, and R. Plomin, “Evidence for Substantial Genetic Risk for Psychopathy in 7-Year-Olds,”
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
46 (2005): 592–97.
    
2
  
a strategy that parents often use with their children:
Martin L. Hoffman,
Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice
(New York: Cambridge University Press, 2000).
    
3
  
a thirteen-year-old mugger:
William Damon,
The Social World of the Child
(San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1977), 18.
    
4
  
Ted Bundy was puzzled:
Quoted in Paul Bloom,
Descartes’ Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human
(New York: Basic Books, 2004).
    
5
  
The serial killer Gary Gilmore summed up the attitude:
Quoted in Bloom,
Descartes’ Baby.
    
6
  
interview with Peter Woodcock:
From Jon Ronson,
The Psychopath Test: A Journey Through the Madness Industry
(New York: Riverhead, 2011), 91.
    
7
  
Some illustrative examples are reported by Charles Darwin:
Charles Darwin, “A Biographical Sketch of an Infant,”
Mind
2 (1877): 285–94.
    
8
  
William responded to the perceived suffering of others:
Darwin, “Biographical Sketch,” 289.
    
9
  
William’s satisfaction at his own kind actions:
Darwin, “Biographical Sketch,” 291.
  
10
  
first hints of guilt and shame:
Darwin, “Biographical Sketch,” 292.
  
11
  
“carefully planned deceit”:
Darwin, “Biographical Sketch,” 292.
  
12
  
“How much money would it take for you to strangle a cat …?”:
Michael Sandel,
Justice: What’s the Right Thing to Do?
(New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2009).
  
13
  
psychopathy can be an asset in business and politics:
Paul Babiak and Robert D. Hare,
Snakes in Suits: When Psychopaths Go to Work
(New York: HarperCollins, 2006).
  
14
  
“the look people get right before I stab them”:
A. A. Marsh and E. M. Cardinale, “Psychopathy and Fear: Specific Impairments in Judging Behaviors That Frighten Others,”
Emotion
12 (2012): 892–98.
  
15
  
Adam Smith didn’t use the word
empathy
 … but he described it well:
Adam Smith,
The Theory of Moral Sentiments
(1759; repr., Lawrence, KS: Digireads.com, 2011), 13.
  
16
  
“my own throat would feel narrow in sympathy”:
John Updike,
Getting the Words Out
(Northridge, CA: Lord John Press, 1988),
17, cited in Elaine Hatfield, John T. Cacioppo, and Richard L. Rapson,
Emotional Contagion
(New York: Cambridge University Press, 1994).
  
17
  
Adam Smith provides another example:
Smith,
Theory of Moral Sentiments
, 13.
  
18
  
mirror neurons:
V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti, “Action Recognition in the Premotor Cortex,”
Brain
119 (1996): 593–609; G. Di Pellegrino, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, and G. Rizzolatti, “Understanding Motor Events: A Neurophysiological Study,”
Experimental Brain Research
91 (1992): 176–80.
  
19
  
comparing it to the discovery of DNA:
V. S. Ramachandran, “Mirror Neurons and Imitation Learning as the Driving Force Behind ‘the Great Leap Forward’ in Human Evolution,” 2009, Edge video, transcript at
www.edge.org/3rd_culture/ramachandran/ramachandran_index.html
.
  
20
  
the initial claims about mirror neurons are significantly overblown:
G. Hickok, “Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in Monkeys and Humans,”
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
21 (2009): 1229–43; Steven Pinker,
The Better Angels of Our Nature: Why Violence Has Declined
(New York: Viking, 2011); Alison Gopnik, “Cells That Read Minds? What the Myth of Mirror Neurons Gets Wrong About the Human Brain,”
Slate
, April 2007,
www.slate.com/articles/life/brains/2007/04/cells_that_read_minds.html
.
  
21
  
empathy exists to motivate compassion and altruism:
For discussion, see C. Daniel Batson,
Altruism in Humans
(New York: Oxford University Press, 2011). For a review of empathy and sympathy from a developmental perspective, see Hoffman,
Empathy and Moral Development.
  
22
  
the link between empathy … is more nuanced than many people believe:
See also J. Prinz, “Is Empathy Necessary for Morality?,” in
Empathy: Philosophical and Psychological Perspectives
, ed. Amy Coplan and Peter Goldie (New York: Oxford University Press, 2010).
  
23
  
Empathy is also influenced by what one thinks of the other person:
T. Singer, B. Seymour, J. P. O’Doherty, K. E. Stephan, R. J. Dolan, and C. D. Frith, “Empathic Neural Responses Are Modulated by the Perceived Fairness of Others,”
Nature
439 (2006): 466–69.
  
24
  
an example from the philosopher Peter Singer of an obviously good act:
P. Singer, “Famine, Affluence, and Morality,”
Philosophy and Public Affairs
1 (1972): 229–43.
  
25
  
the Chinese scholar Mencius:
Quoted in S. Darwall, “Empathy, Sympathy, Care,”
Philosophical Studies
89 (1998): 261–82.
  
26
  
Steven Pinker points out:
Pinker,
Better Angels
, 576.
  
27
  
A real-world case, described by the philosopher Jonathan Glover:
Jonathan Glover,
Humanity: A Moral History of the Twentieth Century
(New Haven: Yale University Press, 2000), 379–80.
  
28
  
Experiments by the psychologist C. Daniel Batson:
C. D. Batson, T. R. Klein, L. Highberger, and L. L. Shaw, “Immorality from Empathy-Induced Altruism: When Compassion and Justice Conflict,”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
68 (1995): 1042–54.
  
29
  
Even newborns respond to other people’s expressions:
A. N. Meltzoff and M. K. Moore, “Imitations of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates,”
Science
198 (1977): 75–78.
  
30
  
parents and babies frequently mirror one another’s expressions:
C. Trevarthen, “The Concept and Foundations of Infant Intersubjectivity,” in
Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny
, ed. Stein Bråten (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1998), 15–46.
  
31
  
the sound of crying is unpleasant for babies; it tends to make them cry themselves:
A. Sagi and M. Hoffman, “Empathic Distress in the Newborn,”
Developmental Psychology
12 (1976): 175–76.
  
32
  
Babies cry more at the sound of another baby’s cry … the cries of a chimpanzee infant:
G. B. Martin and R. D. Clark, “Distress Crying in Infants: Species and Peer Specificity,”
Developmental Psychology
18 (1982): 3–9; M. Dondi, F. Simion, and G. Caltran, “Can Newborns Discriminate Between Their Own Cry and the Cry of Another Newborn Infant?,”
Developmental Psychology
35 (1999): 418–26.

Other books

Cascade by Lisa Tawn Bergren
The Wager by Raven McAllan
Forest Fire by J. Burchett
Crucifax by Garton, Ray
The Armchair Bride by Mo Fanning
Image of the Beast and Blown by Philip Jose Farmer