Read The Mind and the Brain Online

Authors: Jeffrey M. Schwartz,Sharon Begley

Tags: #General, #Science

The Mind and the Brain (24 page)

Although the term
phantom limb
had been around since just after the Civil War, when it was coined by Dr. Silas Weir Mitchell, it had remained a medical conundrum. In 1866 Mitchell had first published his description of it—under a pseudonym. Even when he went public with his finding in 1871, he eschewed the medical journals in favor of the pop magazine
Lippincott’s Journal
, the better to insulate himself from the expected derision of colleagues. The phenomenon has struggled to earn respect, or even recognition of its physical reality. As recently as the 1980s researchers (in the
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry
) ascribed phantom limb to wish fulfillment. Just as one might imagine hearing the voice of a recently
deceased loved one, went their reasoning, so might an amputee feel a recently lost limb.

Ramachandran immediately phoned colleagues in orthopedic surgery and asked whether they had any recent amputees. They did: Victor Quintero, seventeen, who a month before had lost his left arm just above the elbow in a car crash. Victor swore up and down that he could still feel the missing appendage. Ramachandran enlisted him for an experiment. With Victor sitting still with his eyes closed tight, Ramachandran lightly brushed the boy’s left cheek with a cotton swab just as Pons’s team had the Silver Spring monkeys. “Where do you feel that?” Ramachandran asked. On my left cheek, Victor answered—and the back of my missing hand. Stroking one spot on the cheek produced the sensation of his absent thumb’s being touched. Touching the skin between his nose and mouth created the sensation that his phantom index finger was being brushed. The somatosensory remapping was so fine that when Ramachandran stroked a spot just below Victor’s left nostril, the boy felt a tingling on his left pinky. And in perhaps the most peculiar result of somatosensory remapping, when Victor felt an itch in his spectral hand, scratching his lower face produced relief. (Victor was delighted at this, since now, whenever his missing fingers itched, he knew where to scratch.) In a final test, Ramachandran dribbled warm water down Victor’s left cheek—and the young man, incredulous, felt a warm feeling in the ghost of his amputated hand. The feeling was so powerful that he actually double-checked that his arm was still gone.

 

There are some 4 million amputees in the United States. For nearly 70 percent of them their missing arms, hands, legs, or feet continue to experience all-too-real feelings of pressure, pain, warmth, cold, tingling, or other sensations—including Victor’s itching. Human amputees, Ramachandran told a 1993 scientific meeting in Santa Fe, experienced cortical reorganization similar to that found in the Silver Spring monkeys: stimulation of the face
produced an electrical response in both the somatosensory representation of the face and the amputation zone representing the now-missing arm, as if facial nerves had invaded that region. Brain neurons that originally received input from a limb, it seems, react much as the Silver Spring monkeys did to the decrease in sensory input: rewiring themselves to receive input from other sources. Phantom sensation arises from neuroplastic changes in the brain. Neurons in regions that originally fired in response to stimulation of a now-missing body part look for new work, as it were, and instead respond to peripheral neurons that are still in the game. Just as people in Times Square on New Year’s Eve push into any suddenly vacant spot, so surrounding neurons push into the otherwise-silent region of cortex. And also like the New Year revelers, neurons immediately adjacent to a cortical area are most likely to get first dibs at any vacancies.

Which part of the upper quadrant of the body invades the amputation zone therefore turns out to be somewhat random. After a hand is amputated, either the face or the trunk can invade its somatosensory representation. And because the representations of the feet and genitals abut, some people who have suffered the loss of a leg report feeling phantom sensations in the missing limb or limbs during sex: the somatosensory map of the leg, starved of sensation as a result of losing its original input, can be invaded by nerves from the genitals. Similarly, a man whose cancerous penis is amputated may, if his foot is stimulated, have sensations of a phantom penis. (This proximity may help explain why some people find feet erogenous: not merely because the foot unconsciously reminds some people of the penis, as Freud suggested, but also because the somatosensory representation of the foot lies beside the representation of the genitalia.)

The amputation zone, it appeared, was akin to the deafferentation zone in the brains of the Silver Spring monkeys. Monkeys, being somewhat less verbal than your typical amputee, had not been able to tell Pons that cortical remapping produced perceptual
effects. Thus Ramachandran’s was the first report of a living being’s describing the effect of his own brain rewiring.

 

One of those attending the 1993 Santa Fe meeting at which Ramachandran presented his data was Edward Taub. Taub’s rehabilitation into the world of science began in 1986, when Carl McFarland, chairman of the psychology department, recruited him to the University of Alabama, Birmingham (UAB). Taub started work in 1987. The city was trying to shake its history as a citadel of racism and turn itself into a research powerhouse. Taub had an office and a research home. He even had a salary. But he had no “real” money—no research grants. “When I came here I had zero, and not only did I have zero but I couldn’t get anything,” Taub recalls. “It wasn’t the Silver Spring situation,” as he calls it, but the sheer unacceptability of his views on neuroplasticity. Soon after he arrived in Birmingham he gave a presentation on the deafferentation data. After methodically describing how the monkeys would resume using their supposedly useless, deafferented arm if their good arm were constrained, he boldly suggested that a similar approach—constraining the movement of the unaffected arm of stroke patients—might restore the use of the affected arm. After all, there was little to lose. No physical or occupational therapy had really been effective in chronic stroke patients, those whose stroke was years in the past and who were thus past the point of spontaneous recovery.

That amounted to millions of people. Every year, at least 600,000 Americans suffer a stroke, which works out to one victim every fifty-two seconds. Of the 440,000 who do not die immediately, 300,000 are left seriously disabled. Thanks to the graying of America, the personal and social toll from stroke is on the increase, with the prevalence of cerebrovascular accident survivors—the technical term—projected to double by 2050. “I just laid it out, not being antagonistic, and of course I didn’t know anything about the rehabilitation community,” Taub recalls of that first presentation. “I was
stepping on everyone’s toes with this. The head of the rehabilitation center literally began to stammer, and his face became purple, and he said, ‘Are you trying to tell me that a behavioral intervention has an ameliorative effect on a neurological injury of the central nervous system?!’ I said, ‘But, after all, what is physical therapy if not a behavioral intervention?’ He went ballistic. You still have this orientation in the medical community that behavior isn’t real.”

Taub wasn’t the only one whose work connecting plasticity to rehab fell on deaf ears. In 1981 Steve Wolf took up a suggestion Taub had made the year before (Taub himself was still unable to conduct research at this point). Wolf had twenty-five patients with brain damage, most due to stroke, wear a sling on their unaffected arm all their waking hours, except for a half-hour exercise period, for two weeks. He did nothing else. Consistent with Taub’s findings on the deafferented monkeys, however, the patients’ speed and strength of movement in the disabled arm showed significant improvement on lab motor function tests. Although the effect was small (mostly because Wolf did not use intensive training of the patients’ disabled arms), it seemed worth following up. Yet for years no one did. At UCSF, Merzenich and Jenkins had had a similar inspiration. In 1987, they independently proposed that the plasticity of the cortex in response to sensory input, experience, and learning was relevant for stroke rehab. But no one beat down their doors to follow up on the suggestion. After all, “the rehab community was united in opposition to the idea that therapy after a stroke could reverse the neurological effects of the infarct,” Taub recalls. “The official position of the American Stroke Association was that rehab for patients with chronic stroke only increases a patient’s muscular strength and confidence.”

Others were more open-minded. One was the behavioral neuroscientist Niels Birbaumer of Germany’s University of Tübingen. At a 1991 presentation in Munich, he heard Taub propose adapting the therapy he had used on the Silver Spring monkeys—constraining their good arm, forcing them to use their “useless” one—to
stroke patients. Birbaumer invited him to set up a stroke program in Germany. Taub arrived in Tübingen soon after the 1993 Santa Fe meeting and had lunch with the German psychologist Herta Flor. He told her about Ramachandran’s study, describing Ramachandran’s claim that touching the face of someone whose arm has been amputated can evoke the feeling that the missing arm is being touched, and suggested that it needed to be verified. Flor responded, “No problem—why don’t we do it? I’ll just call up my friend Thomas Elbert who has an MEG [
magnetoencephalograph
, which records magnetic changes in neurons that correspond to neuronal activation] and we’ll run some patients.” “I said, ‘fine,’” recalls a still-startled Taub. And thus was born a collaboration that would influence the entire landscape of neuroplasticity. As we learned in Chapter 4, by this time the deafferented Silver Spring monkey research had given rise to two parallel research tracks. One was large-scale cortical reorganization, which Pons and colleagues had put on the map with their experiment on the monkeys. The other was constraint-induced movement (CI) therapy, which as long ago as 1980 had been a glimmer in Taub’s eye, but a glimmer extinguished by the debacle of Silver Spring.

As early as 1987 at least some of Taub’s colleagues at Birmingham had come around to the notion that behavior can leave footprints on the brain, including the injured brain—well, they’d come around enough to collaborate with him. That year Taub and some UAB colleagues began a pilot experiment. They started working with four patients who were in the top quartile of stroke survivors in terms of ability to move their affected arm: they were able to extend their wrist a minimum of twenty degrees and to flex each finger a minimum of ten degrees. Restraining the intact arm of the Silver Spring monkeys or training the deafferented arm had induced the creatures to use that deafferented arm. Taub suspected that the same two procedures applied to a stroke patient would coax movement out of the affected one—especially training the affected arm. The same general techniques that accomplished that
in the deafferented monkeys, Taub maintained, “should be equally applicable following other types of neurological injury, including stroke.”

In constraint-induced movement therapy, stroke patients wear a sling on their good arm for approximately 90 percent of waking hours for fourteen straight days. On ten of those days, they receive six hours of therapy, using their seemingly useless arm: they eat lunch, throw a ball, play dominoes or cards or Chinese checkers, write, push a broom, and use standard rehab equipment called dexterity boards. “It is fairly contrary to what is typically done with stroke patients,” says Taub, “which is to do some rehabilitation with the affected arm and then, after three or four months, train the unaffected arm to do the work of both arms.” Instead, for an intense six hours daily, the patient works closely with therapists to master basic but crucial movements with the affected arm. Sitting across a pegboard from the rehab specialist, for instance, the patient grasps a peg and labors to put it into a hole. It is excruciating to watch, the patient struggling with an arm that seems deaf to the brain’s commands to extend far enough to pick up the peg; to hold it tightly enough to keep it from falling back; to retract toward the target hole; and to aim precisely enough to get the peg in. The therapist offers encouragement at every step, tailoring the task to make it more attainable if a patient is failing, then more challenging once the patient makes progress. The reward for inserting a peg is, of course, doing it again—and again and again. If the patient cannot perform a movement at first, the therapist literally takes him by the hand, guiding the arm to the peg, to the hole—and always offering verbal kudos and encouragement for the slightest achievement. Taub explicitly told the patients, all of whose strokes were a year or more in the past, that they had the capacity for much greater use of their arm than they thought. He moved it for them and told them over and over that they would soon do the same.

In just two weeks of constraint-induced movement therapy with training of the affected arm, Taub reported in 1993, patients
regained significant use of a limb they thought would forever hang uselessly at their side. The patients outperformed control patients on such motor tasks as donning a sweater, unscrewing a jar cap, and picking up a bean on a spoon and lifting it to the mouth. The number of daily-living activities they could carry out one month after the start of therapy soared 97 percent. That was encouraging enough. Even more tantalizing was that these were patients who had long passed the period when the conventional rehab wisdom held that maximal recovery takes place. That, in fact, was why Taub chose to work with chronic stroke patients in the first place. According to the textbooks, whatever function a patient has regained one year after stroke is all he ever will: his range of motion will not improve for the rest of his life.

“It’s true, spontaneous recovery of function usually stops between three and twelve months,” Taub says. But his constraint-induced movement therapy picked up where spontaneous recovery stopped. “We got a large effect in the lab and a huge effect in the life situation,” Taub says. Two years after treatment ended, the constraint patients were still outperforming controls, brushing their teeth, combing their hair, eating with a fork and spoon, picking up and drinking from a glass.

Other books

Sweet Seduction by Nikki Winter
Gathering Water by Regan Claire
Burned by Magic by Jasmine Walt
Hanging Loose by Lou Harper
Smoldering Nights by Carlisle, Lisa
Christmas Fairy by Titania Woods
Lady Rosabella's Ruse by Ann Lethbridge
Tea-Totally Dead by Girdner, Jaqueline
Imbibe! by David Wondrich